Reviews and Overviews Why Olanzapine Beats Risperidone, Risperidone Beats Quetiapine, and Quetiapine Beats Olanzapine: An Exploratory Analysis of Head-to-Head Comparison Studies of Second-Generation Antipsychotics Stephan Heres, M.D. Objective: In many parts of the world,
the results in favor of the sponsor’s drug. John Davis, M.D.
largely replaced typical antipsychotics as
Results: Of the 42 reports identified by Katja Maino, M.D.
nia. Consequently, trials comparing twodrugs of this class—so-called head-to-
head studies—are gaining in relevance.
studies, the reported overall outcome was
Elisabeth Jetzinger, M.D.
in favor of the sponsor’s drug. This pat-
tern resulted in contradictory conclusions
Werner Kissling, M.D.
across studies when the findings of stud-
Stefan Leucht, M.D.
sp o nso rs we re co m p are d. Po te nt ial
and the drug favored in the study’s over-
doses and dose escalation, study entry cri-
teria and study populations, statistics and
Method: The authors identified head-to- head comparison studies of second-gen- Conclusions: Some sources of bias may
limit the validity of head-to-head compar-
ison studies of second-generation antipsy-
bias identified in this review were subtle
all studies fully or partly funded by phar-
rather than compelling, the clinical use-
fulness of future trials may benefit from
mask the names and doses of the drugsused in the trial, and two physicians
minor modifications to help avoid bias.
sources of bias can be addressed by study
initiators, peer reviewers of studies under
were not blinded to the study sponsor re-
viewed the entire report of each study for
(Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:185–194)
A scientific debate about the effectiveness of second- paring the same two drugs have had contradictory conclu-
generation antipsychotics, compared to conventional an-
sions (1, 2). This effect may not be totally unrelated to the
tipsychotics, has been going on for several years. Although
funding sources of the trials. Conflicts of interest arising
all questions have not as yet been answered, second-gen-
from a pharmaceutical company’s sponsorship of clinical
eration antipsychotics are now defined as the gold stan-
trials of a drug it manufactures are obvious (3), and the as-
dard in most aspects of treatment, at least in highly indus-
sociation of funding and conclusions is found in numer-
trialized countries. As a result, so-called head-to-head
ous medical specialties (4). In this article, we present a
comparisons, i.e., randomized, controlled clinical trials
summary of head-to-head comparison studies in psychia-
with two or more active second-generation antipsychotic
try in which we focus on various aspects of potential bias
comparators, have become increasingly important as new
that may arise from such conflicts of interest. To our
knowledge, this work is the first examination of potential
Somewhat confusing is the fact that different trials com-
bias related to study sponsorship of head-to-head com-
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006COMPARISON STUDIES OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS FIGURE 1. Scores Assigned by Two Physicians in Blind Rat-
tion of a trial, unless the abstract stated otherwise. Secondary
ings of 30 Abstracts Reporting the Outcome of Head-to-
publications were excluded in order to avoid multiple inclusions
Head Comparison Studies of Second-Generation Antipsy-
of the source trial in the analysis. We also screened proceedings of
choticsa
selected conferences for the period from 1999 to February 2004. The conference reports we reviewed were limited to materials
from events attended by members of our work group. Blinded Rating of Abstracts
On the basis of the hypothesis that funding by a pharmaceuti-
cal company may influence the outcome of a trial, we checked the
reports for information on sponsorship by a “profit-making orga-nization.” The abstract of each study was modified to mask the
Abstracts 10
names and doses of the drugs used in the trial, and two physicians(a psychiatrist [K.M.] and an internist [E.J.]), both of whom were
blinded to the funding source for the trial and were not involved
in the design of the evaluation, read the complete abstract and
Number of
rated which drug was favored in the overall conclusion. The rat-
ings were made on a 6-point scale proposed by Gilbert et al. (5)
and previously used in studies evaluating the association of fund-ing and conclusions in drug trials (4, 6). The scoring method is de-
scribed in the footnote to Figure 1. For blinding, the second-gen-
eration antipsychotic names in the abstracts were replaced by
“DRUG A” and “DRUG B” (“DRUG A” was not always the sponsor’s
a The abstracts were modified for blind ratings by replacing the drug
drug and vice versa), and the total dose/dose range was replaced
names with “DRUG A” and “DRUG B” and replacing the total dose/
by “x.” A separate sensitivity analysis that included only peer-re-
dose range by “x.” Scores from 4 to 6 favored the sponsor’s drug,
viewed publications was carried out. Two-sided binomial sign
and scores from 1 to 3 favored the comparator. Scores were as-
tests were used to test the hypothesis of potential influence of the
signed according to the following scale: 1=DRUG B is highly pre-
sponsor on the study outcome, and Cohen’s kappa was used for
ferred and is the best alternative; should be considered the stan-
measuring interrater reliability. Statistical significance was de-
dard intervention in all patients, or the like; 2=DRUG B preferred toDRUG A, but DRUG A might be promising under certain circum-
fined at an alpha level of <0.05.
stances or the like; 3=DRUG A and DRUG B about equal, but DRUG
Identifying Potential Sources of Bias
A is disappointing, as DRUG B had some minor advantages; 4=DRUG A and DRUG B about equal, but DRUG A is successful because
The trial reports were read independently by two authors who
of minor advantages; 5=DRUG A preferred to DRUG B, but further
were not blinded to the sponsor of the trial (S.H., S.L.) to identify
trials still indicated; may be more costly or similar disclaimer; 6=
potential sources of bias that could have influenced the results in
DRUG A highly preferred and should be considered the standard in-
favor of the sponsor’s drug. We focused on several factors that
tervention for all patients, or the like. In this example, “DRUG A”
have been discussed as potential sources of bias, including fea-
designates the study sponsor’s drug and “DRUG B” designates the
tures of study design, dose ranges, titration schedules, statistics,
comparator, although in the actual abstracts modified for blind rat-ings, “DRUG A” was not always used to designate the sponsor’s drug
reporting of results, and wording of findings (4, 7, 8). If the con-
clusions of the two reviewers differed, consensus was achieved bydiscussion. The second author (J.D.) checked and approved thefindings. As a reference for dose ranges, we used the following
parison studies of antipsychotic medications. We also ex-
range recommendations included in the American Psychiatric
amined the association of the conclusions of head-to-
Association Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients With
head comparison studies with the source of funding. Con-
Schizophrenia, second edition (9): 10–30 mg/day of aripiprazole,
sequently this study is not a review or a meta-analysis in
150–600 mg/day of clozapine, 10–30 mg/day of olanzapine, 300–
which the efficacy or tolerability of different second-gen-
800 mg/day of quetiapine, 2–8 mg/day of risperidone, 120–200mg/day of ziprasidone, and 5–20 mg/day of haloperidol. For
eration antipsychotics is examined but an exploratory ap-
amisulpride, we used the following dose ranges suggested in the
proach to clarifying partly contradictory study results in
drug company’s product information: 400–800 mg/day for
the field of schizophrenia treatment.
acutely ill patients and 50–300 mg/day for patients with predom-inantly negative symptoms. Search Strategy Search Results
We searched MEDLINE (1966–September 2003) for random-
From 146 publications found in the MEDLINE search,
ized, controlled trials comparing the second-generation antipsy-
we excluded 61 reviews, 22 reports of additional data from
chotics aripiprazole, amisulpride, clozapine, olanzapine, que-tiapine, risperidone, sertindole, and ziprasidone. The search
previously published trials or preliminary results, 17 re-
terms were paired combinations of the second-generation anti-
ports of laboratory or electrophysiological data, five re-
psychotics and the term “rand*” (for “random,” “randomized,”
ports of add-on therapy with other drugs, four reports on
etc.). We excluded reviews, meta-analyses, reports focused solely
alternative diagnoses, 11 reports of studies that did not in-
on laboratory or electrophysiological data, trials with combined
clude a direct head-to-head comparison, and one report
drug treatment, and reports on patient populations with diag-noses other than schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Re-
on combined antipsychotic treatment, which left 25 pub-
ports on drug efficacy were considered to be the primary publica-
lications for analysis. The complete trial report for one of
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006HERES, DAVIS, MAINO, ET AL.
the 25 publications could not be obtained, and that study
TABLE 1. Number of Reports That Favor the Study Spon-
was excluded. Thirteen conference presentations of head-
sor’s Drug or the Comparison Drug in Industry-Sponsored Head-to-Head Comparison Studies of Second-Generation
to-head drug comparisons were identified, and during the
Antipsychotics
analysis, another four publications and one report in press
were identified, for a total of 42 trial reports. Of the 42 re-
ports, 32 were fully or partly funded by a pharmaceutical
company that manufactured one of the drugs used in the
trial (1, 2, 10–39). One of the 42 studies was conductedwith supplemental funding from a pharmaceutical com-
pany, although the acquisition and reporting of the data
were implemented with no input from the company (40);
this study was not included in the blinded rating of ab-
stracts, but it was included in the analysis of sources of
bias. Nine of the 42 studies were not funded by a pharma-
ceutical company (41–49). Two reports of sponsored stud-
ies did not include an abstract (10, 36). Thus, 30 trials were
included in the blinded rating of study abstracts. Sponsorship and Outcome as Reported in Study Abstracts
According to the ratings by the two physicians, the over-
of bias are summarized in a separate table (Data Supple-
all outcome reported in the study abstracts was in favor ofthe sponsor’s drug in 90.0% of the abstracts (N=27 of 30)
ment 1) available from the first author and available with
(p<0.001, binomial sign test) (Figure 1). For each abstract,
the online version of this article at http://ajp.psychiatry-
the scores of the two raters were the same or differed by
online.org. We identified potential sources of bias as de-
only 1 point, and the two raters did not differ in whether
batable or clear. For example, in several instances, we
the outcome was judged to be in favor of the sponsor’s
identified debatable sources of bias in dose ranges for ris-
drug (a score of 4, 5, or 6) or the comparator (a score of 1,
peridone, for which the appropriate range may still be ar-
2, or 3). According to the criteria of Landis and Koch (50),
guable. We identified clear sources of bias in instances in-
the interrater agreement was “moderate” (kappa=0.44,
volving obviously inappropriate choices of dose, design,
p≤0.001) for the numeric rating and “almost perfect”
reporting, etc. We emphasize that although at least some
(kappa=1.0, p<0.001) for the outcome category. Figure 1
of the biases we identified seemed very obvious, our anal-
shows the distribution of the scores for both raters. In the
ysis remains speculative, and there is no proof that the fac-
sensitivity analysis that included only the abstracts that
tors we identified really influenced the results. The biases
underwent peer review (N=21), the result was virtually
we identified are described in the following sections.
identical, with 90.5% (19 of 21) rated as having an outcome
Doses and Dose Escalation
in favor of the sponsor’s drug (p<0.001, binomial sign test). The interrater agreement was “substantial” (kappa=0.61,
Dose ranges and dose escalation are crucial factors that
p<0.001) for the numeric rating and “almost perfect”
potentially influence trial outcome. In numerous trials,
(kappa=1.0, p<0.001) for the outcome category. Table 1
dose ranges are scheduled according to the manufac-
summarizes the ratings for studies comparing pairs of
turer’s package insert, which is problematic with antipsy-
drugs by whether one or the other manufacturer spon-
chotic drugs. For example, in trials with risperidone, doses
sored the study. Only three of these 21 reports did not fa-
up to 10 mg/day or even 12 mg/day are frequently possible
vor the sponsor’s drug. These pairwise comparisons re-
in flexible titration schedules, although this dosing level
vealed contrasting outcomes, depending on the sponsor
may diminish both the efficacy and tolerability of the
of the study, although the outcomes were derived from tri-
drug. After the introduction of risperidone to the market,
several studies in the mid-1990s yielded evidence of anoptimal dose range of 4–8 mg/day, with an increasing risk
Possible Effects of Sponsorship on Trial Outcome
of extrapyramidal side effects at higher doses without any
and Reporting
gain in efficacy (51, 52). At the time of the earliest studies
Two authors who were not blinded to the sponsor of the
included in this summary (1), these data were presumably
trial reviewed the study reports and identified potential
not yet accessible, but in more recent trials, the dose
sources of bias in the following areas: dose and dose esca-
ranges should have been adapted to maintain a fair level
lation, entry criteria and study population, statistics and
of comparison. Trials that did not include the 4-mg/day
methods, and reporting and wording of results. The char-
dose, recently referred to as the advisable dose (53), and
acteristics of the individual trials and the potential sources
trials that allowed doses of up to 12 mg/day (10, 12, 34, 37,
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006COMPARISON STUDIES OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS
40) are problematic. Choosing 4 mg/day as the lower limit
sertindole (11), and ziprasidone (10) became comparators
of the dose range is also problematic, as downward dose
in trials. Exclusion of patients who previously were nonre-
adjustment in case of side effects is not possible. Although
sponders to risperidone or any other comparator (16) is
a dose range of 2–6 mg/day was used in trials sponsored
seldom explicitly stated in reports of head-to-head trials,
by the manufacturer (2, 18), and even lower doses were
although this feature could have a critical effect on obser-
used in elderly patients with schizophrenia in trials spon-
vations of the efficacy of or response to antipsychotic
sored by the manufacturer (19, 21), competitors consis-
For trials involving schizophrenic patients with pre-
Dose ranges are also problematic in comparisons in-
dominantly negative symptoms, questions about the ac-
volving other drugs. Dose ranges of clozapine, especially
curate definition of the study population may be raised.
in trials that included patients with treatment-resistant
Even if appropriate scales for measuring negative symp-
schizophrenia, often appear to be too strictly limited (53),
toms, such as the negative syndrome subscale of the Posi-
resulting in relatively low mean daily doses (<400 mg/day)
tive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) or the Scale for
(13, 14, 39). These levels are in contrast to data revealing
the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, are applied, there
that doses up to 600 mg/day (54) or even 900 mg/day (55,
is still the need for information on positive symptoms, as
56) of clozapine proved highly efficacious in treatment-re-
they might also be present at study entry. An entry crite-
sistant schizophrenia. In comparisons involving olanza-
rion of a difference of 6 points between the PANSS nega-
pine, the upper limit of the dose range is often set at 15
tive and positive subscale scores may ensure that subjects
mg/day (16, 20, 38), thus excluding the most effective 20-
have a predominance of negative symptoms, but it leaves
mg/day dose. Use of this limited dose range possibly re-
room for speculation about the effect of positive symp-
duces olanzapine’s efficacy and may result in a misleading
toms if baseline information about positive symptoms is
conclusion of the competitor’s therapeutic superiority or
not presented (30). Correspondingly, in trials involving pa-
equality. The optimum dose range of amisulpride in pa-
tients with treatment-resistant illness, transparent criteria
tients with predominantly negative symptoms ranges
for inclusion and exclusion of participants are also re-
from 50 to 300 mg/day (57), but in a study comparing
quired (54), although no universally accepted definition of
amisulpride with another antipsychotic, it should have
treatment-resistant schizophrenia exists (59). Studies in
been ensured that the patients did not have significant
which antipsychotic treatment nonresponse and intoler-
positive symptoms at study entry because higher amisul-
ance are allowed as alternative entry criteria (14) may have
pride doses (400–800 mg/day) are necessary for treatment
results that are difficult to interpret. If results derived from
such studies are presented in terms of efficacy in treat-
Finding the optimum dose escalation schedules for
ment-resistant patients, even if the study is not explicitly
both compounds in a study is difficult and may be another
focused on this population, misunderstandings are fore-
source of bias (2, 12, 16, 18–20, 24, 28, 34, 40, 58). In some
cases, the bias may derive from the fact that titration ismandatory for some drugs (risperidone, clozapine, sertin-
Statistics and Methods
dole), while the comparator (for example, olanzapine)
In recent years, studies with a noninferiority design
does not require a stepwise dose escalation. Slow titrationcan prolong the time to the full onset of the therapeutic ef-
have become a reasonable alternative to placebo-con-
fect of a drug, and the optimal dose of the comparator may
trolled trials for comparison of the efficacy of antipsy-
be reached earlier. This difference plays a major role in
chotic agents (60). In a study designed to prove a drug’s su-
studies evaluating efficacy over a brief period of time. On
periority over an active comparator, large sample sizes are
the other hand, side effects might be more likely to appear
usually required. However, equivalence can be shown in a
with fast dose escalation. The attempt to escape the esca-
one-sided noninferiority design with less effort, depend-
lation problem by using a fixed-dose regimen raises other
ing on the predicted threshold for equivalence, although it
problems. Studies with fixed-dose regimens lack natural-
is important to note that in a noninferiority design with a
istic plausibility because the unrealistic limits imposed do
narrow range of equivalence, the sample size required
not reflect the therapeutic flexibility required in the treat-
may exceed that necessary for a superiority design. Conse-
ment of schizophrenia (16, 23, 32, 33, 44, 45).
quently, a basic requirement is to define a priori the extentof the difference between the treatments that is consid-
Entry Criteria and Study Population
ered acceptable for declaring noninferiority (61). It seems
Because the second-generation antipsychotics became
very arguable to assume an equivalent antipsychotic effi-
available on the market one by one over the last decade, a
cacy of a drug at a threshold of just over 60% of the treat-
trial’s entry criteria with respect to previous drug treat-
ment effect achieved by the active comparator as mea-
ment have to be chosen carefully. Risperidone had been in
sured by the reduction in the PANSS total score (10) or the
use for more than 5 years when newer drugs such as
PANSS negative subscale score (30). Other equivalence
amisulpride (32, 37), quetiapine (24, 29), olanzapine (17),
thresholds yield findings of more clinical relevance, but
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006HERES, DAVIS, MAINO, ET AL.
the thresholds differ between comparable studies (28, 32,
provided. If this information is omitted, the reported fre-
quency of occurrence of extrapyramidal symptoms gives
For multiple comparisons, such as those that occur with
only a vague impression of the likelihood of these side ef-
the use of test batteries in cognition studies, an adjust-
ment for multiple testing may be necessary, but no gener-
Poster Reports and Multiple Publishing
ally accepted approach toward this statistical problem ex-ists. One work group may confuse the reader by applying
Phrasing of abstracts is difficult, because much infor-
an adjustment for multiple testing in one study (18, 20)
mation has to be made transparent to the reader in only a
and not in a comparable trial (19). In some studies, the ap-
few lines. Although the abstracts of many head-to-head
plication of an adjustment was not explicitly mentioned or
studies adhere to widely accepted structural standards
adequately discussed, despite the presence of multiple
(65), the results stated are often highly selective. For exam-
ple, in the abstract of one study (29), a significant differ-ence in rates of extrapyramidal symptoms that favored the
Another source of potential bias is a study design in
sponsor’s drug is reported in detail, but the side effects un-
which an acute-phase trial of up to 8 weeks is followed by
favorable to the drug were mentioned without corre-
a continuation phase of up to 12 months that is focused on
long-term maintenance of the treatment effect. After the
Preliminary results of trials are often presented as poster
acute phase, patients who are nonresponders are discon-
reports at conferences. Presentation of multiple poster re-
tinued from the study and only those who meet the re-
ports on the same trial with different first authors can lead
sponse criteria are included in the maintenance phase
to the impression that independent studies have been
(63). This design may be acceptable for relapse studies but
conducted (10, 66). If data from a previously published
leads to problems in response trials. Selecting only re-
trial are later used as the basis for reports focusing on sub-
sponders for continuation in a trial that is focused on re-
populations or secondary objectives, the abstracts of the
sponse (as measured, for example, with the mean reduc-
later studies should contain a cross-reference to disclose
tion of the PANSS score from baseline to endpoint) as well
the source of the data at a glance (62–64, 67, 68). Stand-
as further improvement alters the study population radi-
alone publication of data deriving from another trial with-
cally, necessitating careful interpretation of the results in
out a reference to the earlier trial gives the impression that
separate trials have been conducted (18, 19). Reporting and Wording of Results
A complete disclosure of all results of the head-to-head
Discussion
comparison would appear to be mandatory but is not al-
The first part of our analysis revealed a clear link be-
ways provided. Results favoring the drug manufactured by
tween sponsorship and study outcome as reported in the
the sponsor are often presented in detail, and unfavorable
abstract, as 90.0% of the abstracts were rated as showing
results often are mentioned in a brief sentence at the very
an overall superiority of the sponsor’s drug. This finding is
end of the report’s results section or not mentioned at all
in accordance with numerous previous reports of a similar
(1, 12). Accordingly, the report’s authors may choose to
effect in other medical fields (3, 4, 6, 69). Even more strik-
present only data from observed cases or only data from a
ing were our findings for pair-wise comparison of different
last-observation-carried-forward analysis, depending on
trials that examined the effects of the same two drugs (Ta-
the resulting outcomes. If the last-observation-carried-
ble 1). We found that different comparisons of the same
forward design showed no significant difference between
two antipsychotic drugs led to contradictory overall con-
drugs, the results from the observed cases may be dis-
clusions, depending on the sponsor of the study. On the
played in detail and presented as a significant outcome of
basis of these contrasting findings in head-to-head trials,
the study (11). The relevant population for evaluation of
it appears that whichever company sponsors the trial pro-
the primary outcome should be stated a priori in the pro-
duces the better antipsychotic drug. This peculiar result
tocol and made transparent to the reader.
led us to take a closer look at various design and reporting
Furthermore, reporting of adverse events seems to be
features. Indeed, a number of potential reasons for the as-
selective (34, 36, 38, 62), and the corresponding level of
sociation between drug-company-sponsored trials and fa-
significance for comparisons of rates of adverse events
may not be consistently stated (21, 29). Information onside effects that are very likely to occur, such as sedation
Limitations to Our Approach
and weight gain with olanzapine (15, 64) or elevation of
A first limitation is that we did not retrieve all trials that
prolactin levels with amisulpride (28), may be lacking. In
were presented at conferences. Because no databases for
addition, in reports of extrapyramidal symptoms, detailed
such presentations exist, we were limited to the posters
information on the mean daily dose of anticholinergic
from conferences attended by members of our work
medication and the number of patients who received at
group. The conference presentations we included are
least one dose of anticholinergic medication should be
therefore not necessarily representative of all conference
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006COMPARISON STUDIES OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS
publications. We did not, however, want to exclude this
Sponsorship and outcome as reported in the ab-
material completely, because conferences are an impor-
stract. Our results show that reading only the abstract of
tant way for companies to distribute information. We
a study is insufficient for a complete understanding of the
made no selection among the available reports. The main
study findings. However, lack of time makes it difficult
limitation of our exploratory analysis is that it must re-
even for scientific experts to read all trial reports in detail.
main speculative by nature. Although in some cases—for
Therefore, peer reviewers of studies being considered for
example, the trial in which the optimal risperidone dose of
publication should pay close attention to the conclusions
4 mg/day was explicitly excluded (10)—it is quite obvious
stated in study abstracts. Overall, we found that the struc-
that the factor we identified may have biased the results,
ture of the abstracts in the current review adhered to
there is no proof that it really did. Only a “remake” of the
widely accepted standards (65), but the selection of the re-
study factoring out the source of bias could test the im-
sults and the phrasing used to convey the results needed
pact. Furthermore, other readers may have different opin-
to be carefully scrutinized. To avoid bias in this crucial sec-
ions, especially about the more subtle potential sources of
tion of trial reporting, we suggest that peer reviewers verify
bias. Finally, we emphasize that most of the identified fac-
whether the abstract really summarizes the overall results
tors were indeed rather subtle and did not reflect an at-
of the trial in a balanced way. Detailed guidelines in this
tempt by the drug trial sponsors to intentionally misinter-
area for peer reviewers would be useful.
pret their findings or to willfully mislead readers. Dose and dose escalation. In head-to-head trials, dose Benefit From Industry-Sponsored Trials
ranges and escalation schemes have a major effect on theoutcome. To avoid potential bias, study initiators could
In many respects the industry-sponsored studies in-
ask the competitor to provide a suggested dose range and
cluded in our review met high methodological standards
titration schedule for its compound, as the manufacturer
(26, 27) and often surpassed non-industry-sponsored tri-
of a drug knows its properties best. Alternatively, external
als in the quality of research methods (6, 70). Industry-in-
experts could function as independent advisers, but they
dependent studies are not necessarily free of bias and are
should then be named in the report as a source of infor-
often too underpowered to find statistically significant dif-
mation on the dosing regimen. In addition, responsible
ferences or to allow any generalization (46, 47, 71). In our
agencies such as the U.S. Food and Drug Administration
review, the sample size per group of the nine studies not
(FDA) or the European Medicines Agency (EMEA) might
funded by a for-profit organization ranged from nine to
be given the chance to look at the protocol before the
113 patients. Other factors that contribute to the excellent
study is begun in order to allow the correction of obvious
methodological standards of industry-sponsored trials are
valid central randomization, the high quality of data ac-quisition and management, regular auditing processes,
Entry criteria and study population. Regarding study
and the pharmaceutical company’s researchers’ detailed
population and inclusion criteria, study initiators should
knowledge about the drug (6, 70). There is also no doubt
follow broadly accepted standards in the characterization
that the development of the second-generation antipsy-
of the eligible patients. Diagnostic validity is hardly ever
chotics was a major step forward. For the first time anti-
mentioned in sponsored trials, and theoretically heteroge-
psychotic drugs with clearly defined dose ranges were
neous outcomes may be partly due to the heterogeneity of
made available, while the optimum dose, even of the stan-
the study population. The use of structured clinical inter-
dard conventional antipsychotic haloperidol, is still in
views may help identify the proper study population. For
doubt. Industry-organized trials also markedly improved
example, a characterization of patients with predomi-
our knowledge about general clinical questions such as
nantly negative symptoms has been proposed (73). Defin-
medication switching strategies (72), the treatment of pa-
ing a valid study population is essential in studies of pa-
tients with refractory disorders (34), and the overall effec-
tients with treatment-resistant illness that focus on the
tiveness of new and conventional antipsychotics for treat-
efficacy of antipsychotics, and other aspects of previous
ment of negative symptoms (73). However, if all studies by
treatment discontinuation, such as medication intoler-
drug companies report positive outcomes, the findings
ance, should not be used as alternative inclusion criteria.
Otherwise it is unclear which aspect is related to the supe-riority of a compound (14). Suggestions for Potential Improvement Statistics and methods. A comprehensive assessment
Given the unique opportunities of industry for organiz-
of the statistical methods applied in the studies we re-
ing methodologically sound, large-scale trials, the associa-
viewed is beyond the scope of this article. We therefore
tion between outcome and sponsor found in the rating of
comment only on two points that came up several times
abstracts in our study is unsatisfactory. We believe, how-
during our review. In the last 5 years, noninferiority de-
ever, that in the case of many of the problematic points
signs have become more common, leading to a major
raised in the Results section, relatively simple measures
problem with the threshold of equivalence (74). It is hardly
could improve the situation to an appreciable extent.
acceptable to consider the lower margin of the 95% confi-
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006HERES, DAVIS, MAINO, ET AL.
dence interval at a level of only 60% of the efficacy of the
staff. The international Current Controlled Trials meta-
competitor to be a sign of noninferiority. As the trend to-
register (www.controlled-trials.com) combines national
ward this type of statistical design is likely to endure, an
as well as disease-specific registers, and each trial in-
expert consensus on methods for setting the thresholds is
cluded in the register is assigned a specific number. The
needed. Other confusing aspects include the use of vari-
U.S. Freedom of Information Act mandates publicly ac-
ous test methods and lack of the correction for multiple
cessible “electronic reading rooms” for materials available
statistical tests in trials in which effects on cognitive func-
through the Freedom of Information Act, such as, for ex-
tion are examined. Recently, a guideline for standard test
ample, information about studies registered with the FDA.
batteries for measuring cognition became available (75),
However, in our experience, the registers are not easy to
and it could soon be followed by a consensus on the statis-
tical methods that should be used in this field of research. Poster reports and multiple publishing. Publication
In general, study initiators should define outcome param-
of findings on different aspects of the same trial in several
eters a priori and choose the appropriate correction
reports has been criticized as the “salami strategy” of sci-
method for multiple testing. If the correction method is
entific reporting. This criticism may not always be justi-
applied to a subset of tests only, this fact should be ex-
fied, because it is simply not feasible to report in one pub-
lication all the data from a large trial with several aspects
Reporting and wording of results. Wording and phras-
of interest or a huge sample size. Readers’ understanding
ing of study results are surely the most debatable sources of
of the different aspects covered by the study can be en-
bias. The CONSORT (consolidated standards of reporting
hanced if the masses of data are split into several reports.
trials) statement, developed in the mid-1990s, proposed a
However, authors should always clearly state the source
checklist to ensure completeness of reporting and assess-
reference of the data that are presented (78). Otherwise,
ment of the validity of trial results (76). In addition, the Inter-
the reader might get the impression that several trials were
national Committee of Medical Journal Editors set up a list
undertaken, although in fact there was only one. A similar
of uniform requirements for manuscripts, including trial
problem occurs if different researchers from the same trial
registration and complete reporting of all acquired data (77).
are listed as the first author of various conference presen-
The recommendations leave a considerable margin for
tations or publications by the work group. Because many
wording and interpretation of the findings. Therefore, it is
scientists have only limited time and choose the abstract
again the responsibility of peer reviewers for scientific jour-
as the primary information source, the underlying core
nals to demand balanced reporting of the results.
study should always be mentioned in the abstract. More-over, data presented exclusively in conference poster ses-
Readers of the trial reports should pay close attention to
sions or symposia, which normally do not undergo peer
the choice of the primary outcome variables and to the
review, must be considered problematic (70).
presentation of the results in order to obtain a realistic im-pression of whether a new and unknown aspect of drug
Is It All a Matter of Sponsoring?
treatment, following the “uncertainty principle” (6), was
The need for more industry-independent studies has
observed or whether the study was designed to yield pre-
been recognized, and some have already been conducted
dictable results in favor of the sponsor’s drug. The uncer-
and published (80). Although reports from industry-inde-
tainty principle states that a patient should be enrolled in
pendent trials may not include biased reporting and
a randomized, controlled trial only if there is substantial
wording, specific design features such as dose ranges and
uncertainty about which of the treatments would benefit
study populations can still remain problematic. For exam-
the patient most. For example, the appropriateness of a
ple, the design of a recent industry-independent study of
trial focused on weight gain is debatable if a sponsor’s
Alzheimer’s disease patients (81) has been criticized (82,
drug that is already known for its minor impact on weight
83). The treatment of schizophrenia has many different as-
is compared to a treatment previously shown to be highly
pects, and numerous studies will be needed to advance
treatment. It is unlikely that public funding will cover
The observation that only studies with significant find-
them all. We therefore believe that the chance for further
ings tend to be published led Melander et al. (78) to coin
improvement of current industry-supported trials should
the phrase “evidence b(i)ased medicine.” It is noteworthy
that a guideline for “good publication practice” has beenproposed to help avoid further publication bias (79). Each
Received Nov. 22, 2004; revisions received April 28 and May 31,
protocol registered with the European Clinical Trial Data-
2005; accepted Sept. 19, 2005. From Klinik und Poliklinik für Psychi-
base is issued a unique number, making trials traceable
atrie und Psychotherapie der Technischen Universität München amKlinikum rechts der Isar; the Department of Psychiatry, University of
and missing reports conspicuous. Unfortunately, access to
Illinois at Chicago; the Department of Psychiatry, Ludwig-Maximilian
this information is limited to the study initiator and EMEA
Universität, München, Germany; and the Department of Internal
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006COMPARISON STUDIES OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS
Medicine, Technische Universität München, Germany. Address corre-
16. Cornblatt B: Neurocognitive effects of aripiprazole vs olanza-
spondence and reprint requests to Dr. Heres, Klinik und Poliklinik für
pine in stable psychosis (abstract). Int J Neuropsychopharma-
Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie der Technischen Universität
München am Klinikum rechts der Isar, Moehlstrasse 26, 81675
17. Gureje O, Miles W, Keks N, Grainger D, Lambert T, McGrath J,
München, Germany; s.heres@lrz.tum.de (e-mail).
Tran P, Catts S, Fraser A, Hustig H, Andersen S, Crawford AM:
No source of funding or any grant was used to finance this study.
Olanzapine vs risperidone in the management of schizophre-
Additional information on this study accompanies the online ver-
nia: a randomized double-blind trial in Australia and NewZealand. Schizophr Res 2003; 61:303–314
18. Harvey PD, Green MF, McGurk SR, Meltzer HY: Changes in cog-
References
nitive functioning with risperidone and olanzapine treatment:a large-scale, double-blind, randomized study. Psychopharma-
1. Tran PV, Hamilton SH, Kuntz AJ, Potvin JH, Andersen SW, Beas-
ley C Jr, Tollefson GD: Double-blind comparison of olanzapine
19. Harvey PD, Napolitano JA, Mao L, Gharabawi G: Comparative
versus risperidone in the treatment of schizophrenia and other
effects of risperidone and olanzapine on cognition in elderly
psychotic disorders. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1997; 17:407–418
patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Int J
2. Conley RR, Mahmoud R: A randomized double-blind study of
risperidone and olanzapine in the treatment of schizophrenia
20. Harvey PD, Siu CO, Romano S: Randomized, controlled, dou-
or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:765–
ble-blind, multicenter comparison of the cognitive effects of
ziprasidone versus olanzapine in acutely ill inpatients with
3. Bekelman JE, Li Y, Gross CP: Scope and impact of financial con-
schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Psychopharmacol-
flicts of interest in biomedical research: a systematic review.
21. Jeste DV, Barak Y, Madhusoodanan S, Grossman F, Gharabawi
4. Als-Nielsen B, Chen W, Gluud C, Kjaergard LL: Association of
G: International multisite double-blind trial of the atypical an-
funding and conclusions in randomized drug trials: a reflection
tipsychotics risperidone and olanzapine in 175 elderly patients
of treatment effect or adverse events? JAMA 2003; 290:921–
with chronic schizophrenia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003; 11:
5. Gilbert JP, McPeek B, Mosteller F: Statistics and ethics in surgery
22. Breier A, Berg PH, Thakore JH, Naber D, Gattaz WF, Cavazzoni P,
and anesthesia. Science 1977; 198:684–689
Walker DJ, Roychowdhury SM, Kane JM: Olanzapine versus
6. Djulbegovic B, Lacevic M, Cantor A, Fields KK, Bennett CL, Ad-
ziprasidone: results of a 28-week double-blind study in pa-
ams JR, Kuderer NM, Lyman GH: The uncertainty principle and
tients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1879–
industry-sponsored research. Lancet 2000; 356:635–638
7. Bodenheimer T: Uneasy alliance—clinical investigators and
23. Kinon B: Improvement of comorbid depression with olanza-
the pharmaceutical industry. N Engl J Med 2000; 342:1539–
pine versus ziprasidone treatment in patients with schizophre-
nia or schizoaffective disorder, in Abstracts of the XIIth Biennial
8. Safer DJ: Design and reporting modifications in industry-spon-
Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos, Switzerland, Feb 7–
sored comparative psychopharmacology trials. J Nerv Ment Dis
13, 2004. Schizophr Res 2004; 67(suppl 1):163
24. Knegtering R, Castelein S, Bous H, Van Der LJ, Bruggeman R,
9. American Psychiatric Association: Practice Guideline for the
Kluiter H, van den Bosch RJ: A randomized open-label study of
Treatment of Patients With Schizophrenia, second edition. Am
the impact of quetiapine versus risperidone on sexual func-
tioning. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2004; 24:56–61
10. Addington D, Pantelis C, Dineen M, Bermattia I, Romano SJ,
25. Lecrubier Y, Bouhassira M, Olivier V, Lancrenon S, Crawford
Murray SR: Ziprasidone versus risperidone in schizophrenia: 52
AM: Olanzapine versus amisulpride and placebo in the treat-
weeks of comparative data, in 2003 Annual Meeting New Re-
ment of negative symptoms and deficit states of chronic
search Program and Abstracts. Arlington, Va, American Psychi-
schizophrenia, in Abstracts of the 12th Congress of the Euro-
pean College of Neuropsychopharmacology, London, Sept 21–
11. Azorin J, Toumi M, Sloth-Nielsen M: Sertindole is well tolerated
25, 1999. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1999; 9(suppl 5):288
and superior to risperidone with respect to efficacy in patients
26. McQuade RD, Jody D, Kujawa MJ, Carson WH Jr, Iwamoto T,
with schizophrenia (abstract). Schizophr Res 2003; 60(suppl 1):
Archibald DG, Stock EG: Long-term weight effects of aripipra-
zole versus olanzapine, in 2003 Annual Meeting New Research
12. Azorin J-M, Spiegel R, Remington G, Vanelle J-M, Péré J-J,
Program and Abstracts. Arlington, Va, American Psychiatric As-
Giguere M, Bourdeix I: A double-blind comparative study of
clozapine and risperidone in the management of severe
27. Meltzer HY, Alphs L, Green AI, Altamura AC, Anand R, Bertoldi
chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1305–1313
A, Bourgeois M, Chouinard G, Islam MZ, Kane J, Krishnan R,
13. Bitter I, Dossenbach MR, Brook S, Feldman PD, Metcalfe S,
Lindenmayer JP, Potkin S: Clozapine treatment for suicidality in
Gagiano CA, Furedi J, Bartko G, Janka Z, Banki CM, Kovacs G,
schizophrenia: International Suicide Prevention Trial (In-
Breier A: Olanzapine versus clozapine in treatment-resistant or
terSePT). Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:82–91
treatment-intolerant schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychophar-
28. Mortimer A, Martin S, Loo H, Peuskens J: A double-blind, ran-
domized comparative trial of amisulpride versus olanzapine
14. Bondolfi G, Dufour H, Patris M, May JP, Billeter U, Eap CB, Bau-
for 6 months in the treatment of schizophrenia. Int Clin Psy-
mann P (Risperidone Study Group): Risperidone versus cloza-
pine in treatment-resistant chronic schizophrenia: a random-
29. Mullen J, Jibson MD, Sweitzer D: A comparison of the relative
ized double-blind study. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:499–504
safety, efficacy, and tolerability of quetiapine and risperidone
15. Ciudad A: Olanzapine and risperidone: results of a one-year
in outpatients with schizophrenia and other psychotic disor-
randomized trial in outpatients with schizophrenia with prom-
ders: the Quetiapine Experience With Safety and Tolerability
inent negative symptoms, in Abstracts of the XIIth Biennial
(QUEST) study. Clin Ther 2001; 23:1839–1854
Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos, Switzerland, Feb 7–
30. Olie JP, Spina I, Benattia I: Ziprasidone versus amisulpride in
13, 2004. Schizophr Res 2004; 67(suppl 1):161
the treatment of negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a 12-
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006HERES, DAVIS, MAINO, ET AL.
week, double-blind trial (abstract). Schizophr Res 2002;
45. Klieser E, Lehmann E, Kinzler E, Wurthmann C, Heinrich K:
Randomized, double-blind, controlled trial of risperidone ver-
31. Oliemeulen EAP: Is olanzapine a substitute for clozapine? the
sus clozapine in patients with chronic schizophrenia. J Clin Psy-
effects on psychomotor performance, in Abstracts of the 10th
Biennial Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos, Switzer-
46. Svestka J: A double-blind comparison of olanzapine and que-
land, Feb 5–11, 2000. Schizophr Res 2000; 41(1):187
tiapine in the treatment of acute exacerbations of schizo-
32. Peuskens J, Bech P, Moller HJ, Bale R, Fleurot O, Rein W: Amisul-
phrenic disorders, in Abstracts of the 16th Congress of the Eu-
pride vs risperidone in the treatment of acute exacerbations of
ropean College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Prague, Sept
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1999; 88:107–117
20–24, 2003. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003; 13(suppl 4):
33. Potkin SG, Saha AR, Kujawa MJ, Carson WH, Ali M, Stock E,
Stringfellow J, Ingenito G, Marder SR: Aripiprazole, an antipsy-
47. Svestka J: Olanzapine versus risperidone in first-episode schizo-
chotic with a novel mechanism of action, and risperidone vs
phrenic and schizoform disorders: a double-blind comparison,
placebo in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective dis-
in Abstracts of the 16th Congress of the European College of
order. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:681–690
Neuropsychopharmacology, Prague, Sept 20–24, 2003. Eur
35. Ritchie CW, Chiu E, Harrigan S, Hall K, Hassett A, Macfarlane S,
Neuropsychopharmacol 2003; 13(suppl 4):S291
Mastwyk M, O’Connor DW, Opie J, Ames D: The impact upon
48. Wahlbeck K, Cheine M, Tuisku K, Ahokas A, Joffe G, Rimon R:
extra-pyramidal side effects, clinical symptoms and quality of
Risperidone versus clozapine in treatment-resistant schizo-
life of a switch from conventional to atypical antipsychotics
phrenia: a randomized pilot study. Prog Neuropsychopharma-
(risperidone or olanzapine) in elderly patients with schizophre-
nia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003; 18:432–440
49. Daniel DG, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Kleinman JE, Pickar D,
34. Purdon SE, Jones BD, Stip E, Labelle A, Addington D, David SR,
Lubick LJ, Williams TS: Different side effect profiles of risperi-
Breier A, Tollefson GD: Neuropsychological change in early
done and clozapine in 20 outpatients with schizophrenia or
phase schizophrenia during 12 months of treatment with olan-
schizoaffective disorder: a pilot study. Am J Psychiatry 1996;
zapine, risperidone, or haloperidol. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;
50. Landis JR, Koch GG: The measurement of observer agreement
36. Sacchetti E: Comparison of quetiapine, olanzapine and risperi-
for categorical data. Biometrics 1977; 33:159–174
done in patients with schizophrenia: interim results of a ran-
51. Chouinard G, Jones B, Remington G, Bloom D, Addington D,
domized, rater-blinded study, in Abstracts of the 16th Congress
MacEwan GW, Labelle A, Beauclair L, Arnott W: A Canadian
of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, Pra-
multicenter placebo-controlled study of fixed doses of risperi-
gue, Sept 20–24, 2003. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003;
done and haloperidol in the treatment of chronic schizo-
phrenic patients. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1993; 13:25–40
37. Sechter D, Peuskens J, Fleurot O, Rein W, Lecrubier Y: Amisul-
52. Peuskens J: Risperidone in the treatment of patients with
pride vs risperidone in chronic schizophrenia: results of a 6-
chronic schizophrenia: a multi-national, multi-centre, double-
month double-blind study. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;
blind, parallel-group study versus haloperidol. Br J Psychiatry
38. Simpson GM, Weiden PJ, Pigott TA, Romano SJ, Siu C: Ziprasi-
53. Davis JM, Chen N: Dose response and dose equivalence of an-
done versus olanzapine in schizophrenia: 6-month blinded
tipsychotics. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2004; 24:192–208
continuation study, in 2002 Annual Meeting New Research
54. Kane J, Honigfeld G, Singer J, Meltzer H: Clozapine for the treat-
Program and Abstracts. Arlington, Va, American Psychiatric As-
ment-resistant schizophrenic: a double-blind comparison with
chlorpromazine. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:789–796
39. Tollefson GD, Birkett MA, Kiesler GM, Wood AJ: Double-blind
55. Simpson GM, Josiassen RC, Stanilla JK, de Leon J, Nair C, Abra-
comparison of olanzapine versus clozapine in schizophrenic
ham G, Odom-White A, Turner RM: Double-blind study of clo-
patients clinically eligible for treatment with clozapine. Biol
zapine dose response in chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychia-
40. Volavka J, Czobor P, Sheitman B, Lindenmayer J-P, Citrome L,
56. Rosenheck R, Cramer J, Xu W, Thomas J, Henderson W, Frisman
McEvoy JP, Cooper TB, Chakos M, Lieberman JA: Clozapine,
K, Fye C, Charney D: A comparison of clozapine and haloperi-
olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in the treatment of
dol in hospitalized patients with refractory schizophrenia. N
patients with chronic schizophrenia and schizoaffective disor-
41. Jerrell JM: Cost-effectiveness of risperidone, olanzapine, and
57. Leucht S, Pitschel-Walz G, Engel RR, Kissling W: Amisulpride, an
conventional antipsychotic medications. Schizophr Bull 2002;
unusual “atypical” antipsychotic: a meta-analysis of random-
ized controlled trials. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:180–190
42. de Haan L, Beuk N, Hoogenboom B, Dingemans P, Linszen D:
58. Thornley B, Adams C: Content and quality of 2000 controlled
Obsessive-compulsive symptoms during treatment with olan-
trials in schizophrenia over 50 years. Br Med J 1998; 317:1181–
zapine and risperidone: a prospective study of 113 patients
with recent-onset schizophrenia or related disorders. J Clin Psy-
59. Miller AL, Hall CS, Buchanan RW, Buckley PF, Chiles JA, Conley
RR, Crismon ML, Ereshefsky L, Essock SM, Finnerty M, Marder
43. Breier AF, Malhotra AK, Su TP, Pinals DA, Elman I, Adler CM, La-
SR, Miller DD, McEvoy JP, Rush AJ, Saeed SA, Schooler NR, Shon
fargue RT, Clifton A, Pickar D: Clozapine and risperidone in
SP, Stroup S, Tarin-Godoy B: The Texas Medication Algorithm
chronic schizophrenia: effects on symptoms, parkinsonian side
Project antipsychotic algorithm for schizophrenia: 2003 up-
effects, and neuroendocrine response. Am J Psychiatry 1999;
date. J Clin Psychiatry 2004; 65:500–508
60. Laster LL, Johnson MF: Non-inferiority trials: the “at least as
44. Conley RR, Kelly DL, Richardson CM, Tamminga CA, Carpenter
good as” criterion. Stat Med 2003; 22:187–200
WT Jr: The efficacy of high-dose olanzapine versus clozapine in
61. D’Agostino RB, Massaro JM, Sullivan LM: Non-inferiority trials:
treatment-resistant schizophrenia: a double-blind crossover
design concepts and issues—the encounters of academic con-
study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2003; 23:668–671
sultants in statistics. Stat Med 2003; 22:169–186
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006COMPARISON STUDIES OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS
62. Sajatovic M, Mullen JA, Sweitzer DE: Efficacy of quetiapine and
73. Moller HJ: Amisulpride: efficacy in the management of chronic
risperidone against depressive symptoms in outpatients with
patients with predominant negative symptoms of schizophre-
psychosis. J Clin Psychiatry 2002; 63:1156–1163
nia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001; 251:217–224
63. Wirtz HS, Kinon BJ, Zhao Z, Barber B: Acute response to olanza-
74. Gomberg-Maitland M, Frison L, Halperin JL: Active-control clin-
pine but not to risperidone predicts the likelihood of contin-
ical trials to establish equivalence or noninferiority: method-
ued improvement over time in patients with schizophrenia
ological and statistical concepts linked to quality. Am Heart J
(abstract). Schizophr Res 2002; 53(suppl 1):181
64. Edgell ET, Andersen SW, Johnstone BM, Dulisse B, Revicki D,
75. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, Barch DM, Cohen J, Es-
Breier A: Olanzapine versus risperidone: a prospective compar-
sock S, Fenton WS, Frese F, Goldberg TE, Heaton RK, Keefe RS,
ison of clinical and economic outcomes in schizophrenia.
Kern RS, Kraemer H, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Zalcman S,
Marder SR: Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clin-
65. Haynes RB, Mulrow CD, Huth EJ, Altman DG, Gardner MJ: More
ical trials in schizophrenia: the NIMH-MATRICS conference to
informative abstracts revisited. Ann Intern Med 1990; 113:69–
select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biol Psychiatry 2004;
66. Benattia I, Addington D, Pantelis C, Dineen M, Murray S:
76. Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman DG: The consort statement: re-
Ziprasidone versus risperidone in schizophrenia: an eight-
vised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of
week, double-blind trial with forty-four-week continuation (ab-
parallel-group randomised trials. Clin Oral Investig 2003; 7:2–7
stract). Schizophr Res 2003; 60(suppl 1):273
77. ICMJE (International Committee of Medical Journal Editors):
67. Feldman PD, Kaiser CJ, Kennedy JS, Sutton VK, Tran PV, Tollef-
Uniform Requirements for Manuscripts Submitted to Biomedi-
son GD, Zhang F, Breier A: Comparison of risperidone and olan-
cal Journals: writing and editing for biomedical publication.
zapine in the control of negative symptoms of chronic schizo-
phrenia and related psychotic disorders in patients aged 50 to
78. Melander H, Ahlqvist-Rastad J, Meijer G, Beermann B: Evidence
65 years. J Clin Psychiatry 2003; 64:998–1004
b(i)ased medicine—selective reporting from studies sponsored
68. Tollefson GD, Andersen SW, Tran PV: The course of depressive
by pharmaceutical industry: review of studies in new drug ap-
symptoms in predicting relapse in schizophrenia: a double-
plications. Br Med J 2003; 326:1171–1173
blind, randomized comparison of olanzapine and risperidone.
79. Wager E, Field EA, Grossman L: Good publication practice for
pharmaceutical companies. Curr Med Res Opin 2003; 19:149–
69. Montgomery JH, Byerly M, Carmody T, Li B, Miller DR, Varghese
F, Holland R: An analysis of the effect of funding source in ran-domized clinical trials of second generation antipsychotics for
80. Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Byerly MJ, Glick ID, Canive JM,
the treatment of schizophrenia. Control Clin Trials 2004; 25:
McGee MF, Simpson GM, Stevens MC, Lieberman JA: The Na-
tional Institute of Mental Health Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of
70. Lexchin J, Bero LA, Djulbegovic B, Clark O: Pharmaceutical in-
Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project: schizophrenia trial
dustry sponsorship and research outcome and quality: system-
design and protocol development. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:
atic review. Br Med J 2003; 326:1167–1170
71. Casey DE, Carson WH, Saha AR, Liebeskind A, Ali MW, Jody D, In-
81. Courtney C, Farrell D, Gray R, Hills R, Lynch L, Sellwood E, Ed-
genito GG: Switching patients to aripiprazole from other anti-
wards S, Hardyman W, Raftery J, Crome P, Lendon C, Shaw H,
psychotic agents: a multicenter randomized study. Psychop-
Bentham P: Long-term donepezil treatment in 565 patients
with Alzheimer’s disease (AD2000): randomised double-blind
72. Leucht S, Pitschel-Walz G, Abraham D, Kissling W: Efficacy and
extrapyramidal side-effects of the new antipsychotics olanza-
82. Holmes C, Burns A, Passmore P, Forsyth D, Wilkinson D:
pine, quetiapine, risperidone, and sertindole compared to
AD2000: design and conclusions. Lancet 2004; 364:1213-1214
conventional antipsychotics and placebo: a meta-analysis of
83. Akintade L, Zaiac M, Ieni JR, McRae T: AD2000: design and con-
randomized controlled trials. Schizophr Res 1999; 35:51–68
Am J Psychiatry 163:2, February 2006
info_villagordiani@podisticapreneste.it Moroni Roberto <moroni.r@fonspa.it> Inviato: info@podisticapreneste.it; info_villagordiani@podisticapreneste.it Oggetto: Partecipazione alla 2^ Giro del lago di BraccianoCiao ragazzi, i dati della mia partecipazione. Ci aggiungo anche una cronaca dell'avvenimento. 64 - MORONI ROBERTO G.S. POD. PRENESTE - 34 giorni ieri in